Tone (music)
A musical tone is the sound produced by a musical instrument playing a particular musical note.
A laboratory determination of pitch is made by a subject listening to a tone from a musical instrument and to the tone of a single sinusoidal wave of one frequency (such as that produced by a tuning fork), and identifying for what frequency the instrument and the sinusoid sound alike.[2] However, the tone of an instrument is determined only partially by pitch. The differences in tone between musical instruments can be analyzed using Fourier series to identify the frequencies comprising the tone.[3]
The frequency spectrum of a musical instrument playing a particular note varies with the instrument and with the way that it is played. The manner of playing determines the sound envelope of a note, and therefore the amplitude of its constituent frequencies.[1]
Some instruments (like the flute or the violin) exhibit a fundamental frequency and multiples of that frequency called harmonics, all of various amplitudes and phases, and others (like the cymbal or the drum) do not.[3][4]
These ideas find application in the construction of music synthesizers and musical instruments of all kinds.
Helmholtz' theory
Hermann Ludwig F. von Helmholtz developed a theory for the perception of musical sounds.[5] However, apart from his ideas about perception, which involve conjectures about how the ear operates, Helmholtz commented upon the production of sound by musical instruments.
To understand Helmholtz' comments below, some vocabulary is needed: A single note is often referred to as a tone, the lowest frequency present as the fundamental or prime partial tone and harmonics of this frequency that appear when a note is played on an instrument as harmonic upper partial tones or upper tones or partial tones. Where only one frequency is present, as with a tuning fork, it is called a simple tone:[5]
It is well known that this union of several simple tones into one compound tone, which is naturally effected in the tones produced by most musical instruments, is artificially imitated on the organ by peculiar mechanical contrivances. The tones of organ-pipes are comparatively poor in upper partials. When it is desirable to use a stop of incisive penetrating quality of tone and great power, the wide pipes (principal register and weithgedackt) are not sufficient; their tone is too soft too defective in upper partials; and the narrow pipes (geigen-register and quintaten) are also unsuitable, because, although more incisive, their tone is weak. For such occasions, then, as in accompanying congregational singing, recourse is had to the compound stops. In these stops every key is connected with a large or smaller series of pipes, which it opens simultaneously, and which give the prime tone and a certain number of the first upper partials of the compound tone of the note in question. Helmholtz, p. 91[5] |
These words identify the organ as a form of mechanical rather than electronic music synthesizer. They also lay out the characterization of a musical note by its constituent frequency components, in the same way that a Fourier series expresses a periodic waveform in terms of its harmonic components.
Fourier series
As the remarks of Helmholtz suggest, the sound of a note played upon each instrument is characterized by not only its fundamental tone or frequency, which is the same for all instruments, but by the higher partial tones or harmonics in the note, which are different in amplitude and in phase for each instrument, making them sound different.
A musical note is a a periodic function in time and in space, modulated by a sound envelope. That is, the note can be expressed in one spatial dimension as:
where s is the sound envelope that expresses the duration of the note at a fixed location or its extent in space at a fixed time, and f is a periodic function of its argument:
with λ the wavelength of f and T = λ/v the period of f and v the speed of propagation of f through space.
The periodic factor in such a wave can be analyzed using a Fourier series to show it is made up of a summation of sinusoidal waves, each with their own frequency. These frequency components are useful in distinguishing the different characteristics of the same note played upon different instruments, as shown approximately in the figure. The spectrum is altered somewhat by the sound envelope, so the musician has some control over the tone.
Timbre
Naturally, besides the tone produced by the instrument, the musical experience also depends upon how the ear responds to the tone, and how the tone is modified by the auditorium where it propagates.[6] These effects that relate to the perception of this sound are separate from the production of sound by the instrument. The perceived quality of a tone is referred to as timbre. The American Standards Association definition of timbre describes it as "that attribute of auditory sensation in terms of which a listener can judge that two sounds similarly presented and having the same loudness and pitch are dissimilar", and a note to this definition adds that "timbre depends primarily upon the spectrum of the stimulus, but it also depends upon the waveform, the sound pressure, the frequency location of the spectrum, and the temporal characteristics of the stimulus" [7]
Chord
As a musical tone consists of a fundamental tone and higher partial tones, one might ask how this differs from a chord, that is, several tones played simultaneously. This question is fundamentally one of perception: for example, how is it that a note played on a musical instrument is heard as a certain timbre, while several notes sound not like a note played upon a different instrument (a note of single timbre), but like a chord (a combination of notes with separate timbres)? Apparently the difference has to do with the loudness of the constituent frequencies, and the fusion of separate harmonics into a single whole is disturbed when some component frequencies gain an intensity comparable to the fundamental, resulting in the perception of a chord.[8]
References
- ↑ 1.0 1.1 Stanley R. Alten (2010). “Sound envelope”, Audio in Media, 12th ed. Cengage Learning, p. 13. ISBN 049557239X.
- ↑ Thomas D. Rossing (2007). Springer Handbook of Acoustics. Springer, p. 477. ISBN 0387304460.
- ↑ 3.0 3.1 Leon Gunther (2011). The Physics of Music and Color. Springer, p. 47 ff. ISBN 1461405564.
- ↑ Bart Hopkin (1996). “Figure 2-4”, Musical Instrument Design: Practical Information for Instrument Making. See Sharp Press, p. 110. ISBN 1884365086.
- ↑ 5.0 5.1 5.2 Hermann Ludwig F. von Helmholtz (1875). On the sensations of tone as a physiological basis for the theory of music, Translation by Alexander John Ellis. Oxford University Press, p. 33.
- ↑ David Martin Howard, Jamie Angus (2009). “§3.1.2 Musical notes and their harmonics”, Acoustics and Psychoacoustics, 4rth ed. Focal Press, pp. 124 ff. ISBN 0240521757.
- ↑ American Standard Acoustical Terminology S1.1-1960, p. 45. This description is much quoted: see Petri Toiviainen (1996). "Optimizing auditory images and distance metrics for self-organizing timbre maps". Journal of Music Research 25: 1-30. DOI:10.1080/09298219608570695. Research Blogging. and Robert Erickson (1975). Sound Structure in Music. University of California Press, p. 4. ISBN 0520023765.
- ↑ Robert Erickson (1975). “Pitch (with timbre) → chord”, Sound Structure in Music. University of California Press, p. 20. ISBN 0520023765.