Monitor theory: Difference between revisions

From Citizendium
Jump to navigation Jump to search
imported>John Stephenson
(expanded intro)
imported>John Stephenson
(moving section to subpage)
Line 2: Line 2:
''Not to be confused with the 'monitor hypothesis', one component of monitor theory.''
''Not to be confused with the 'monitor hypothesis', one component of monitor theory.''


'''Monitor theory''' refers to five hypotheses<ref>e.g. Krashen (1981); see also Markee (1997: 25).</ref> developed by the [[linguist]] [[Stephen Krashen]] to explain [[second language acquisition]] (SLA):
'''Monitor theory''' refers to five hypotheses<ref>e.g. Krashen (1981); see also Markee (1997: 25).</ref> developed by the [[linguistics|linguist]] [[Stephen Krashen]] to explain [[second language acquisition]] (SLA):


*the ''[[#Acquisition versus learning|acquisition-learning]]'' hypothesis;
*the ''[[#Acquisition versus learning|acquisition-learning]]'' hypothesis;
Line 37: Line 37:
==Footnotes==
==Footnotes==
{{reflist|2}}
{{reflist|2}}
==External links==
*[http://sdkrashen.com sdkrashen.com] Some of Stephen D. Krashen's books and articles, available on-line.
*[http://homepage.ntlworld.com/vivian.c/SLA/Krashen.htm Krashen's Comprehension Hypothesis Model of L2 learning] [[applied linguistics|Applied linguist]] Vivian Cook's page on Krashen's hypotheses.

Revision as of 03:17, 4 August 2009

This article is developing and not approved.
Main Article
Discussion
Related Articles  [?]
Bibliography  [?]
External Links  [?]
Citable Version  [?]
 
This editable Main Article is under development and subject to a disclaimer.

Not to be confused with the 'monitor hypothesis', one component of monitor theory.

Monitor theory refers to five hypotheses[1] developed by the linguist Stephen Krashen to explain second language acquisition (SLA):

Monitor theory, as defined by Krashen, states that adult learners have two systems to enable them to develop their language ability: subconscious acquisition and conscious learning, with acquisition being more important. Conscious learning is only available as a "monitor", i.e. learners can consciously 'edit' their 'output' (utterances or written work) to make themselves more fluent or comprehensible, based on what they have formally learned about the second language. This, however, has no effect on subconscious, true acquisition, and in its absence, output will be less 'accurate' or native speaker-like.[2] Each of the hypotheses relate to conditions that are necessary for acquisition to take place within a system that involves subconscious emergence of language alongside conscious monitoring: for example, the natural order hypothesis points towards a fairly fixed sequence of acquisition that adult language users go through when monitoring does not interfere much, and which is closer to first language acquisition by children.

Acquisition versus learning

The acquisition-learning distinction is the most fundamental of these and the most widely known among linguists. According to Krashen these are two independent systems of L2 performance; acquisition is a product of subconscious processing similar to children’s L1 acquisition and requires life-like L2 interaction, which focuses on communication rather than correctness, while learning occurs through formal instruction and comprises conscious processing, which results in knowledge about the L2, e.g. grammatical rules. Krashen believes ‘learned competence’ acts as a monitor or editor: that is, whereas ‘acquired competence’ is responsible for the fluent production of sentences, ‘learned competence’ consciously corrects them. He claims that learned knowledge enables learners to read and listen more, so acquisition is effective.

Monitoring

The monitor hypothesis asserts that a learner's learned system acts as a monitor to what they are producing. In other words, while only the acquired system is able to produce spontaneous speech (according to this theory), the learned system is used to check what is being spoken. The interlocuter therefore monitors their spontaneous speech using their learned system. The Monitor Model then predicts faster initial progress by adults than children, as adults use this ‘monitor’ when producing L2 utterances before having acquired the ability for natural performance, and adults will input more into conversations earlier than children. However, in the long term, SLA started in childhood will be superior in ultimate attainment as children will already have control of some L2 acquired before pubertal changes began inhibiting learning.

Input

The input hypothesis states that only comprehensible input will result in acquisition of the target language. Krashen says that learners must be exposed to input that is just beyond their current level in order to make progress. This concept is called i+1. If the level of input is at i+1 the learner will make progress. If it is too high, for instance i+7, the learner will be unable to acquire it.

Affect

The affective filter hypothesis asserts that a learner's emotional states act as adjustable filters that freely permit or hinder input necessary to acquisition. He suggests that adolescence and puberty are not good periods for SLA, as this ‘affective filter’ arises out of self-conscious reluctance to reveal oneself and feelings of vulnerability.

Criticism

The model has proved controversial since its appearance in the early 1980s. Though its hypothesis would seem to be supported by many linguists and teachers - e.g. that lots of input is necessary, that there is a difference between acquisition and learning, etc.[3] - it has also been strongly criticised due to the prevailing mood in applied linguistics that learned knowledge does form part of true acquisition.[4]

McLaughlin (1987) claims that none of the hypotheses is clear in its prediction, for example, the acquisition-learning distinction is not properly defined and the distinction cannot be empirically tested. If only acquired forms can lead to spontaneous speech, as Krashen claims, then it should be impossible for anyone who learns a foreign language in a classroom, and is taught it in their native language, to ever be able to produce spontaneous speech in the target language. This is clearly untrue. Likewise, Krashen provides no criteria for establishing i+1, or for delineating different levels of input. Similarly, the monitor hypothesis and the affective filter hypothesis are not falsifiable either. For one thing, the value of a monitor that only notices mistakes after they are produced is questionable. Furthermore, there is no way of determining how the monitor works or proving if it is even there at all. If a learner produces a correct form in the target language, it is impossible to determine what caused them to produce that form. There is no way to prove whether it was their acquired system or their learned system, if there is even a distinction between the two. Similarly, there is no way to prove how the affective filter hypothesis filter works. The affective filter hypothesis also fails to take into account why a motivated learner, whose affective filter should be down, could still have trouble learning a language.

However, continued interest in Krashen’s theory indicates this theory is far from pseudo-scientific. In determining an optimal SLA age, the variables these theories propose are not the only possibilities: affective factors, including motivation and fear, also influence language-learning attitudes. These factors can explain maturational differences in SLA: older learners often learn new languages for economic or academic reasons, and therefore work harder to reach their target fluency.

The Natural Approach

The theory underlies Krashen and Terrell's comprehension-based language learning methodology known as the natural approach (1983).

Footnotes

  1. e.g. Krashen (1981); see also Markee (1997: 25).
  2. Krashen (1981: 1-2).
  3. See e.g. Scrivener (2005:19), a handbook for teachers which points out that the alternative of a strong focus on explicit instruction has proved of little help to beginning learners.
  4. See Gregg (1984) for a strong critique.